Skip to main content

Power asymmetry and embarrassment in shared decision-making: predicting participation preference and decisional conflict

Abstract

Background

Shared decision-making (SDM) is the gold standard for patient-clinician interaction, yet many patients are not actively involved in medical consultations and hesitate to engage in decisions on their health. Despite considerable efforts to improve implementation, research on barriers to SDM within the patient-clinician relationship and interaction is scant. To identify potential barriers to urological patients’ participation in decision-making, we developed two novel scales assessing power asymmetry (PA-ME) and embarrassment in medical encounters (EmMed). The present study validates both scales in a large sample comprising urological patients and non-clinical participants. It further examines the effects of both factors on participation preferences and decisional conflict among patients.

Methods

Data were collected from 107 urological patients at a university hospital for Urology and Urosurgery in Germany. Patients completed self-report questionnaires before and after their clinical appointments. In addition, 250 non-clinical participants provided data via an online study. All participants rated perceived power asymmetry in the patient-clinician relationship and their experience of embarrassment in medical contexts using the PA-ME and EmMed scales. Urological patients further indicated their participation preference in decisions regarding both general and urological care prior to the consultation. Afterward, they assessed the level of perceived decisional conflict.

Results

Factor analyses yielded power asymmetry and medical embarrassment as unidimensional constructs. Both questionnaires have good (PA-ME; α = 0.88), respectively excellent (EmMed; α = 0.95), internal consistency. Among urological patients, higher levels of perceived power asymmetry predicted lower generic participation preference (β = − 0.98, p <.001, adjusted R2 = 0.14) and higher decisional conflict (β = 0.25, p <.01, adjusted R2 = 0.07). While, in patients, embarrassment was not linked to generic participation preference before the consultation (p ≥.5), it resulted in higher decisional conflict after the consultation (β = 0.39, p <.001, adjusted R2 = 0.14). Neither power asymmetry nor embarrassment were specifically associated with participation preference regarding urological care (p ≥.273).

Conclusions

Given their promising psychometric properties, the new instruments are recommended for routine assessment of power asymmetry and embarrassment among patients. Addressing these factors may be helpful to reduce decisional conflict and increase participation preferences. Both factors are prerequisites for a successful SDM-process and active patient engagement in health-related decisions.

Peer Review reports

Background

Shared decision-making (SDM) is the gold standard of patient-clinician interaction [1] and is particularly important for preference-sensitive decisions [2]. In such preference-sensitive decisions, treatment options show comparable efficacy [3, 4], and the benefit-harm ratio depends on patients’ individual priorities and capacities [4, 5]. Consequently, their preferences must be explicitly inquired and considered in the decision-making process [6].

From a practical perspective, SDM positively impacts various patient outcomes, such as knowledge [7, 8], treatment adherence [9,10,11], and treatment satisfaction [12,13,14]. Beyond that, it strengthens and respects patients’ right to self-determination and autonomy within medical consultations [1, 15, 16]. Consequentially, SDM has been incorporated into international policy agendas, patient rights, and healthcare education and training [17]. In Germany, SDM is part of the National Cancer Plan, which particularly calls for the practice of SDM in oncological consultations [18].

Moreover, a large proportion of oncological patients prefer SDM in their treatments, with rates ranging from 33% [19] to 46% [20]. Among tumor entities, urooncological patients exhibit particularly high preferences for active participation [20, 21]. Specifically, patients with cancer of the male genital organs demonstrate the highest desire towards autonomy in decision-making [19]. This may be explained by the several preference-sensitive, high-stake decisions urooncological patients face before and during treatment [22, 23]. In the case of early-stage prostate cancer, for example, patients and clinicians must carefully consider the patients’ capacities and preferences when choosing among alternative treatment choices, including active surveillance, surgery, or various forms of radiation treatment [24, 25]. Thus, promoting patients’ autonomy in urological decision-making is crucial, and implementation of SDM is demanded by current urological and urooncological treatment guidelines [26,27,28]. Consistently, a survey among German urologists indicated that 84% consider SDM the preferred method of patient-clinician communication [29].

However, so far individual patient characteristics or treatment preferences are only partially addressed in oncological care [30]. The percentage of patients who report experiencing SDM ranges from 39% [20] to 51% [19] in oncology populations and around 45% [20, 31] in urooncological settings. Furthermore, despite the significant proportion of cancer patients favoring SDM, about a quarter prefer to not be involved in health decisions [19, 20].

Therefore, a significant step to enhance the implementation of SDM is to identify and address barriers that impede patients’ willingness to engage in health decisions. To date, efforts have primarily focused on the development of clinician guidance and structural requirements, as well as institutional care programs that center on the principles of SDM [17, 32, 33]. However, the inherent dynamics of the patient-clinician relationship and potential impediments within their interactions have been notably overlooked. The patient-clinician relationship is naturally characterized by an imbalance, as patients seek help, expertise, and care that can only be provided by clinicians [34]. However, the perception of power asymmetry and feelings of embarrassment may intensify perceived inferiority and inadequacy among patients, hindering them from expressing preferences, voicing disagreements, and actively participating in the decision-making process. While qualitative patient interviews have yielded initial support for this notion [35], yet to date, there is limited quantitative empirical research on these interactional impediments.

Power asymmetry as a barrier to SDM

The inherently imbalanced relationship between help-seeking patients and care-providing clinicians [34] has been found to be further reinforced by two critical assumptions held by patients [35]. First, the belief that a “good” patient should be passive and compliant [35]. Often, patients think that active engagement in consultations might be seen as undermining the clinicians’ authority, possibly straining the relationship [36]. Second, patients tend to underestimate their capacity to comprehend medical information provided by the clinician [37] and mistakenly assume that their preferences and personal information are superfluous to the decision-making process [35].

Both assumptions may lead to deferring decisions to the clinician and hinder patients’ engagement in health discussions. Indeed, power asymmetry has been found to prevent patients from speaking up - even when they possess a high level of medical knowledge [36] or have severe concerns about the received quality of care [38,39,40]. This effect has previously been called white-coat silence [41] and is particularly pronounced when the stakes are high, as in the case of severe diseases and heightened dependency on the clinician [1, 42].

Given the life-threatening nature of urooncological conditions, patients in this field likely perceive heightened vulnerability and dependency in their relationship with clinicians. Consequently, this patient population may be especially prone to experiencing power asymmetry, making them particularly relevant for assessing the impact of power asymmetry on engagement in SDM. Indeed, an initial study by Büdenbender et al. [24] found that urological and urooncological patients’ beliefs about their role and acceptable behavior in the interaction and decision-making process were strong predictors of their participation preference in decision‐making. The study used a prior measure of power asymmetry, predominantly examining patients’ positive and negative beliefs about participating in decision-making. This includes thoughts about the advantages of participating, competency beliefs, trust in the clinician, and clinicians’ reactions to participation [43]. However, the scale misses out on patients’ perceived dependency on the clinician, the avoidance of speaking up, and how patients value a good relationship with the clinician. Consequently, to facilitate the assessment of the prevalence and impact of power asymmetry in everyday health practice, we developed the Power Asymmetry in Medical Encounters (PA-ME) questionnaire (details on the scale development are provided in the Measures section).

Embarrassment in medical encounters

In addition to power asymmetry, embarrassment may not only emerge as a highly relevant emotion in the medical context but is also likely to impede patient-clinician communication. As a self-conscious emotion [44], embarrassment typically arises when there is a discrepancy between an individuals’ self-presentation and the perceived societal standard for self-presentation [45]. Distinguished from shame, which is elicited in response to morally wrong or reprehensible actions [46], embarrassment is a response to something that threatens our projected image but is otherwise morally neutral. While both terms are often used interchangeably in the research context of medical encounters, this study specifically concentrates on the nuanced aspects of embarrassment.

Given the intimate and socially sensitive nature of urology, examinations in this specialty are likely to elicit embarrassment in patients. Procedures often involve intimate body areas and require patients to undress, which can elicit substantial embarrassment in patients [47, 48]. Invasive procedures such as urinary catheterization to monitor excretory functions further thread patients’ boundaries and dignity [49]. Beyond that, consultations can encompass intimate or stigmatized topics like the patients’ sexual practices [50], unhealthy lifestyles, or past non-compliance with health advice [49]. Consequently, urological patients appear to be a suitable population to assess the prevalence and impact of embarrassment in medical encounters.

However, empirical research on the occurrence and impact of embarrassment in general medical encounters and urology specifically is rare [51, 52]. Initial studies on patients suffering from urinary incontinence show that noticeable symptoms, like urine odor, wet pants, or frequent daytime urination, induce embarrassment in patients [53, 54]. Crucially, results are limited by their reliance on single-item assessments or qualitative interviews.

Previous measures to quantitatively assess medical embarrassment cover elicitors as the appearance and function of the body, intimate medical examinations, and concerns about judgment or negative evaluation by healthcare providers [55, 56]. However, embarrassment stemming from a lack of knowledge about medical terms, past non-compliance with treatment, or sharing private information has not been addressed. In response to this, we introduce the Embarrassment in Medical Consultation (EmMed) questionnaire to provide a comprehensive assessment of patients’ experiences with medical embarrassment (details on the scale development are provided in the Measures section). Once validated, the EmMed questionnaire promises to allow for an assessment of medical embarrassment prevalences in urology and facilitates investigations into its impact on SDM. When discussing concerns about medication side effects on sexual functioning, an initial study found embarrassment as a notable impediment to effective communication and information seeking [57]. However, their results were limited by their reliance on qualitative interviews, and the focus on a different medical specialty. Thus, to particularly understand the effect of embarrassment on SDM in urology, further research with a validated psychometric measure is needed.

Aim of the study and hypotheses

First, we aim to evaluate two novel measures of relevant dynamics within patient-clinician interaction. Specifically, we assess the factor structure and internal consistency of the two newly developed questionnaires on power asymmetry and embarrassment in medical encounters in a large dataset comprising a urological patient sample and a non-clinical sample. In the second step, we investigate how both constructs, power asymmetry and embarrassment, relate to urological patients’ preference towards participation in decisions regarding their general and urological care. As it is a commonly used patient-reported outcome measure of SDM quality [58,59,60], we further assess both constructs’ relationship to patients’ perceived decisional conflict after consultation.

Methods

Participants and procedure

Urological sample

Data was collected at the emergency room and the elective outpatient clinic of the Department for Urology and Urosurgery at University Medical Center Mannheim, Germany. Eligible participants were at least 18 years old and fluent in German. A nurse or research assistant approached patients and informed them about the study. After providing informed consent, patients completed a set of self-report questionnaires before and after their appointment with a clinician. While waiting for their appointment (pre-consultation), patients provided sociodemographic data and filled in the German version of the Autonomy Preference Index (API; [61, 62]), the Autonomy Preference Index for Urooncology (API-Uro; [63]) and the newly developed questionnaires on power asymmetry (PA-ME, see Appendix A, Additional File 1) and embarrassment (EmMed, see Appendix B, Additional File 1) in medical encounters. After the consultation, patients were instructed to continue with the German version of the Decisional Conflict Scale (DCS; [64]). Beyond that, clinical information was obtained from the patients’ electronic health records. The study protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the Medical Faculty of Mannheim, University of Heidelberg (amendment to MA-2019–635N). Data collection took place from September 2021 to February 2023. When a study nurse or research assistant was available, patients were invited to participate. This process did not involve any systematic selection beyond the listed eligibility criteria.From an initial recruitment of n = 128 urological patients, 21 participants (16.4%) were excluded from analyses: Seven were excluded due to substantial missing data (≥ 60%), three because they had no consultation with a clinician on the day of data acquisition, and another 11 patients due to potentially systematic response patterns. Hence, the final sample consisted of n = 107 urological patients. Given the absence of any systematic pattern of missing values in the left-over sample, we used median imputation in cases where a maximum of two items per questionnaire were missing.

The sample was primarily male (n = 86 men, 80.4%), of older age (M = 61.2, SD = 12.1), and of German nationality (n = 100, 93.5%). Moreover, 42.1% were oncological patients, and another 18.7% had a tentative cancer diagnosis. Further details on sociodemographic and clinical characteristics, as well as sample comparisons, are reported in Table 1.

Table 1 Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of urological and non-clinical sample and results of sample comparisons

Non-clinical sample

To ensure that both scales could be validated in a large and diverse sample, we additionally collected data from a non-clinical sample online via SoSci Survey [65]. The study was advertised on social media, the online research platform SurveyCircle [66], and the local survey system of the University of Mannheim [67]. After giving informed consent, participants provided sociodemographic data and filled in the EmMed and PA-ME questionnaires. In the questionnaires, participants do not need to relate their responses to a current illness or treatment. They are rather asked to express their overall attitudes towards the patient-clinician relationship and involvement in medical decision-making, and their general embarrassment experiences in the medical context, respectively. Thus, we enlarged our sample to validate the novel questionnaires by also administering them to a non-clinical population sample. Data collection was carried out between March 2021 and June 2021.

Of n = 265 participants, 15 (5.7%) were excluded due to not passing the attention control item. Hence, the final sample consisted of n = 250 subjects. Participants were primarily female (n = 199 women, 79.6%), of young age (M = 26.6, SD = 9.5), and of German nationality (n = 236, 94.4%). See Table 1 for further details.

Measures

Power asymmetry in medical encounters (PA-ME)

The PA-ME assesses the patients’ perceived power asymmetry in their relationship with the clinician. Items were developed based on previous research [35, 43, 68] and theoretical considerations regarding elicitors and consequences of perceived power asymmetry. Items were then revised with feedback from urological patients (Selbsthilfe-Bund Blasenkrebs e. V.) [self-help association for bladder cancer]. The examined 20-item version of the questionnaire is reported in Appendix A, Additional File 1. Items assess feelings of social inferiority, patients’ expectations and assumptions about clinicians’ reactions, avoidance behavior caused by perceived power asymmetry, and how important the relationship to and perception by clinicians is to the patients. Items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Sum scores are calculated and transformed with min-max normalization to range from 0 to 100 (PA-ME Score) for comparability to other scores in the research field (e.g. API-Uro; [63]). Higher values indicate higher levels of perceived power asymmetry.

Embarrassment in medical consultation (EmMed)

The EmMed assesses perceived embarrassment elicited by lack of medical knowledge, appearance and function of the body, medical examinations, stigmatization of diseases, intimate questions, and missing treatment compliance in the past. Items were developed based on prior research [55, 69] and theoretical considerations regarding the elicitors and effects of embarrassment in medical encounters. After consulting with several psychologists and urologists, items were revised with feedback from urological patients (Selbsthilfe-Bund Blasenkrebs e. V.) [self-help association for bladder cancer]. The examined 21-item version of the questionnaire is reported in Appendix B, Additional File 1. Items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Sum scores are calculated and transformed with min-max normalization to range from 0 to 100 (EmMed Score), with higher values indicating a higher level of perceived embarrassment.

In addition to the EmMed questionnaire, we exploratorily assessed whether the participant has ever shown health-related avoidance behavior (for example, skipped a health appointment or an examination) because of embarrassment provoked by any of the mentioned elicitors (7 items). Items were answered in a dichotomous response format (yes vs. no). With no referring to 0 and yes referring to 1, a sum score was built. Ranging from 0 to a maximum of 7, higher scores indicate more avoidance behavior due to embarrassment in the past. Furthermore, we used a single item to exploratorily assess whether the participants’ decision-making was ever impaired by perceived embarrassment. The item is presented with a 5-point Likert response format ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), with higher values indicating more decision impairment. Exploratory results are reported in Appendix C, Additional File 1.

Autonomy preference index (API)

We used the German API questionnaire’s decision-making preference subscale (API-dm; four inverted items) to assess patients’ generic participation preference [62]. The subscale is commonly used on its own and has been validated in several settings [70, 71]. Responses are made on a Likert scale ranging from 0 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). Sum scores are min-max normalized to range from 0 to 100, with higher values indicating a stronger desire for autonomy. In previous studies, the internal consistency of the German API-dm subscale was high (α = 0.81, [72]; α = 0.85, [62]). This study’s internal consistency was comparable (urological sample: α = 0.82).

Autonomy preference index for urooncology (API-Uro)

The API-Uro [63] consists of four vignettes and seven belonging items. Each item represents a putative decision about essential steps in urological or urooncological care ranging from initial diagnosis through critical treatment decisions to follow-up care. Patients indicate who should make the corresponding decision on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (physician alone) to 5 (patient alone). Scores of 3 indicate a preference for shared decisions. An additional control item assesses whether patients were able to put themselves in the position of the patient described in the vignettes. A sum score is calculated and transformed with min-max normalization to a range from 0 to 100 for easier interpretation. Higher values indicate a preference for more autonomy. In previous studies, the internal consistency of the scale was satisfying (α = 0.83, [72]; α = 0.92, [63]). In this study’s urological sample, internal consistency was comparable (α = 0.87).

Decisional conflict scale (DCS)

We used the German version of the DCS [64] to assess perceived uncertainty about which option to choose and decisional conflict after consultation. The questionnaire consists of 16 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 (strongly agree) to 4 (strongly disagree). Sum scores are built, and min-max normalized to range from 0 to 100, with higher values indicating a higher level of decisional conflict and unfavorable decisions. A score ≥ 25 marks a significant decisional conflict associated with decision delay, regret, lower treatment compliance, and a higher intention to sue clinicians in harm cases [73, 74]. The psychometric properties of the DCS have been acceptable in previous samples (average α = 0.88 in a review of 54 studies, [75]). In this study, internal consistency in the urological sample was satisfying (α = 0.96).

Statistical analyses

We utilized a large dataset collected in the urological sample and the non-clinical sample (N = 357) to conduct polychoric exploratory factor analyses (EFAs) to assess the factorial validity of the PA-ME questionnaire and the first subscale of the EmMed questionnaire. Analyses were calculated based on polychoric correlations, which are most appropriate for ordinal-scaled variables [76], and were used for validation of further SDM-related measurements [63]. The eligibility of data for EFA was checked with the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sample adequacy (KMO; cut-offs 0.6 = mediocre, 0.7 = middling, 0.8 = meritorious, 0.9 = marvelous; [77]) and the Bartlett’s test of sphericity. Subsequently, we followed best practice recommendations [78,79,80] and calculated the EFAs using the minimal residual (minres) extraction method with an oblique rotation (oblimin). The optimal number of factors to retain was determined by comparing results of the Scree test, parallel analysis, very simple structure (VSS) criterion, and Velicer’s minimum average partial (MAP) test [79, 80]. The resulting dimensions were built by calculating sum scores. Their reliability is reported in terms of ordinal coefficient α [81, 82].

To investigate the role of power asymmetry and embarrassment as potential barriers to SDM among urological patients, we conducted linear regressions. Specifically, we predicted participation preference regarding decisions in (1) general and (2) urological care and (3) perceived decisional conflict among the urological sample with perceived power asymmetry and embarrassment, respectively. For participation preferences regarding urological care, the sample consisted of patients who stated they could put themselves in the patient’s position described in the vignettes (cutoff: ≥ 4 in control item of the API-Uro). Statistical significance was set at α = 0.05. Where applicable, we report effect sizes [83]. All analyses were conducted in RStudio version 2023.06.2 + 561 [84].

Results

Psychometric evaluation of questionnaires

Power asymmetry in medical encounters (PA-ME)

Data suitability for conducting the EFA was confirmed with the KMO test of sampling adequacy (KMO = 0.84, meritorious; [77]) and a significant Barlett’s test, χ²(190) = 2030.93, p <.001. As the KMO value of one item was < 0.7, it was removed from analyses [85]. All remaining items had a KMO value greater than 0.7, range = [0.71; 0.92]. Thus, no further item was removed.

Results of parallel analysis based on polychoric correlations (for the polychoric correlation matric, see Appendix D, Additional File 1) suggested five factors, but in larger samples parallel analysis likely resembles over-factoring [86]. Results of the Scree test, the VSS criterion, and Velicer’s MAP test allowed for either a one- or a three-factor-solution. As the three-factor-dimensions demonstrated poor internal consistencies and consisted of few items each, we decided to extract one factor, resulting in 28% of explained variance in the PA-ME items. Factor loadings of the items are presented in Table 2. Two further items have been removed due to low factor loadings (≤ 0.3). The standardized ordinal coefficient α of the resulting factor was good (α = 0.88). The final version of the questionnaire will be available in German and English on MADOC (University of Mannheim, https://madoc.bib.uni-mannheim.de/) Research Output Repository.

Table 2 Polychoric factor loadings of the PA-ME questionnaire items (in decreasing order of loadings)

Embarrassment in medical consultations (EmMed)

Data suitability of the items for calculating the EFA was confirmed by a marvelous KMO measure of sampling adequacy (KMO = 0.93; [77]) and a significant Bartlett’s test, χ²(210) = 3777.51, p <.001. All items had a KMO value greater than 0.7, range = [0.89; 0.96], thus no item was removed from analyses [85].

Parallel analysis based on polychoric correlations (for the polychoric correlation matric, see Appendix E, Additional File 1) suggested a five-factor structure. However, because in larger samples parallel analysis likely resembles over-factoring [86], we decided to retain one factor in concordance with results of the Scree test, the VSS criterion, and Velicer’s MAP test. The extraction of one factor explained 47% of variance in the EmMed items. Factor loadings are presented in Table 3. The standardized ordinal coefficient α was excellent (α = 0.95). The final version of the questionnaire will be available in German and English on MADOC (University of Mannheim, https://madoc.bib.uni-mannheim.de/) Research Output Repository.

Table 3 Polychoric factor loadings of EmMed questionnaire items (in decreasing order of loadings)

Prediction of SDM by power asymmetry and embarrassment in urological patients

Descriptive statistics of power asymmetry and embarrassment assessed in both samples are reported in Table 4. In addition, descriptive statistics of the SDM-process variables (API, API-Uro, DCS) assessed in the urological sample are reported. Pearson product-moment correlations of all measures are reported in Appendix F, Additional File 1.

Table 4 Descriptive statistics of primary variables in urological and non-clinical sample and results of sample comparisons

Power asymmetry

Higher levels of power asymmetry significantly predicted lower participation preference (API), β = − 0.98, t(101) = -4.21, p <.001, and explained a substantial proportion of variance with an adjusted R2 = 0.14, F (1,101) = 17.7, p <.001 (see Fig. 1). Furthermore, higher levels of power asymmetry significantly predicted greater decisional conflict (DCS), β = 0.25, t(88) = 2.76, p =.007, and explained a meaningful proportion of variance, adjusted R2 = 0.07, F (1,88) = 7.62, p =.007 (see Fig. 1). Power asymmetry was no significant predictor for participation preference regarding urological care (API-Uro) (p =.825) (see Fig. 1).

Embarrassment

Higher levels of embarrassment significantly predicted more decisional conflict (DCS), β = 0.39, t(88) = 3.92, p <.001, and explained a substantial proportion of variance, with an adjusted R2 = 0.14, F (1,88) = 15.36, p <.001. However, embarrassment was not a significant predictor for participation preference in general (API, p =.567) and regarding urological care (API-Uro, p =.273) (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Prediction of SDM variables by power asymmetry and embarrassment in the urological sample

Note. Predictions were conducted with linear regression. Bold lines and p-values indicate significant predictions

Discussion

SDM is the gold standard of patient-clinician interaction, but several barriers can get in the way. We introduced two short questionnaires to assess power asymmetry and embarrassment in medical encounters. With promising reliability, the questionnaires can be used in routine medical care to assess both constructs in patients. This is crucial given our findings that (a) urological and non-clinical populations perceive power asymmetry and embarrassment in clinical encounters, and (b) both dynamics significantly impact relevant prerequisite and outcome variables of the SDM process. We found that patients with limiting assumptions about power asymmetry are less inclined to engage in SDM and experience more decisional conflict following consultation with their clinician. Moreover, those who report being embarrassed will later experience more decisional conflict after consultation.

The PA-ME questionnaire demonstrated promising internal consistency, surpassing previous measures (standardized ordinal coefficient α = 0.88 vs. Cronbach’s α = 0.72 in [43]). By incorporating items on additional aspects, the introduced questionnaire comprehensively evaluates perceived power asymmetry, addressing facets untouched by earlier tools [43]. Exploratory analyses suggested the presence of additional dimensions in the PA-ME questionnaires. However, they exhibited poor internal consistencies and were characterized by a reduced number of items. Consequently, we decided to model the questionnaire as a unidimensional measurement of power asymmetry. Further research is crucial to fully understand and differentially explore the facets our research hinted at. As our sample was highly heterogeneous and included urological and non-clinical participants, future analyses with separate, more homogeneous samples will enhance our understanding of power asymmetry’s factor structure, ensuring the questionnaire’s validity across diverse populations.

Similarly, the EmMed questionnaire exhibits excellent internal consistency and covers elicitors of embarrassment not covered by previous measures to assess patient embarrassment [55]. We did not identify distinct dimensions of medical embarrassment, as previously observed in other assessments [55]. Confirmatory studies with homogenous study populations and further exploratory studies including the questionnaire sections on related avoidance behavior and decision-making impairment will be valuable in further exploring the structure and effects of medical embarrassment.

Finally, the PA-ME and EmMed questionnaires facilitate the routine assessment of power asymmetry and embarrassment within patients, enabling its effective addressing during consultations. This is highly recommended, given our findings on their substantial impact on the SDM process and decision-making outcomes.

Power asymmetry as a barrier to SDM in urological patients

In our patient sample, mean scores of both participation preference scales indicate that patients preferred SDM as decision-making model in general and urological care [61, 62]. However, perceived power asymmetry predicted a decrease in generic participation preference among patients. This highlights the profound impact even subtle power imbalances might have and emphasizes the relevance of this variable to the implementation of SDM in medical consultations. This is also supported by the alignment of our findings with initial research on the effects of power asymmetry on patients’ autonomy preferences [24, 35, 87].

Moreover, our data yielded an effect of power asymmetry on decisional conflict among patients after their consultation with a clinician. This suggests that power imbalances not only decrease patients’ participation preferences but might discourage patients from engaging in decision-making during their appointments. This, in turn, may result in decisions that are less aligned with patients’ values and preferences, as indicated by our findings. The majority of patients in our study reported moderate levels of decisional conflict (Muro = 12.06, SD = 11.28), which is in line with previous findings (N = 313, M = 18.02, SD = 17.47; [59]). Still, 21.5% of the patients we surveyed scored at or above the threshold for significant decisional conflict, which is linked to delaying decisions and experiencing regret [72]. This further underlines the need to address barriers that predict patient decisional conflict. Importantly, while we assessed participation preferences before the consultation and decisional conflict as an outcome of decision-making after the consultation, we did not evaluate the actual occurrence of SDM during the consultation. As this work has identified that embarrassment and power asymmetry influence the outcome of decision-making, future research should address the mechanisms of power asymmetry during the actual consultation with the physician.

We did not find a significant prediction of participation preference regarding urological care (API-Uro) through power asymmetry. Urological patients may have specific concerns about their treatment options before consultation and feel a greater need to address them. Given the significant impact of urological, and particularly urooncological, conditions on patients’ lives [25] patients may feel a greater awareness of the importance of engaging in decision-making. This, in turn, may result in a less inhibited preference to participate in urological treatment decisions, in contrast to the need to participate in decisions in general.

Embarrassment as a barrier to SDM in urological patients

Our findings suggest that patients who experience embarrassment in medical encounters will later report more decisional conflict after consultation with their clinicians. However, interestingly embarrassment did not impact patients’ preferences for participating in decisions regarding general or urological care, which aligns with prior findings using the same outcome measure [88]. However, Hamann et al. [88] found that embarrassment indeed links to less critical and participatory communication among patients. As these are crucial prerequires, this may ultimately hinder SDM during consultations. This converges with our finding that embarrassment among urological patients predicts greater decisional conflict after counseling: Without ruling out their participation preference, embarrassment may impede patients’ ability to express their preferences effectively, leading to decisions that do not align with patients’ priorities and increased decisional conflict. High or moderate levels of participation preferences do not necessarily mean more participation, and a mismatch between preferred and actual involvement is not uncommon [19]. Our exploratory finding on moderate scores of decision-making impairments due to embarrassment in both samples (see Appendix C, Additional File 1) further supports this hypothesis. Future research should consider and test these assumptions. To gain deeper insights, future research should also incorporate measurements of SDM occurrence and patient participation during the consultation.

Power asymmetry and embarrassment are both dynamics that come into play in the direct interaction and communication between patients and clinicians and can be regarded as predominantly interpersonal barriers to SDM. Also, both barriers contribute to patients’ feelings of insufficiency and inferiority. While the primary focus of this study was not to investigate their specific relationship, we discovered a significant positive correlation between both constructs, r(105) = 0.56, p <.001 (see Appendix F, Additional File 1), suggesting that both barriers are closely related. It is thus plausible that power asymmetries also affect embarrassments’ impact on communication. When patients initially feel embarrassed, their willingness to engage in SDM may increase if the clinician is attentive to their concerns and open to involving them in the decision. However, embarrassed patients who interact more passively are likely to have less influence on their treatment, which can perpetuate power imbalances. Conversely, interventions that address power asymmetry, embarrassment, or patient empowerment in general may have mutually reinforcing effects, resulting in improved overall health outcomes [89] and well-being [23].

Limitations and outlook

We assessed the impact of power asymmetry and embarrassment in a urological and urooncological sample, as we expected this patient group to be especially prone to experiencing both barriers. Our patient sample was representative of urological and urooncological patients, being primarily male and aged over 60 [90]. However, to generalize our findings to other patient groups, the questionnaires should be validated in further patient samples.

To validate our questionnaires, we calculated the EFAs with a large and diverse dataset comprising patient and non-clinical participants. This allowed for increasing data variability and higher correlation coefficients [91]. Yet, there may be differences in factor structures between patient and non-clinical samples. The limited sample size of the urological sample did not allow separate analyses. However, exploratory analyses with the non-clinical sample only revealed comparable suggestions for factorial structures (see Appendix G, Additional File 1). Nevertheless, sample-specific factorial structures of both questionnaires should be further examined in future studies. The limited sample size of urological patients may also have hindered us from detecting smaller effects due to a lack of statistical power. Future studies with larger sample sizes may uncover further links between both barriers and the SDM process, particularly regarding the relationship between embarrassment and participation preferences.

We found varying levels of power asymmetry and embarrassment among patients and non-clinical participants. However, samples differed significantly regarding sociodemographic characteristics. Also, while patients sought medical advice for serious urological and oncological conditions, non-clinical participants did not currently face health threats. Thus, although our questionnaires evaluate general attitudes and experiences, caution is needed in interpreting sample differences. Nevertheless, patients may possess extensive knowledge regarding their condition and treatment options, greater familiarity with medical procedures, and a better understanding of what to expect in medical encounters than non-clinical individuals. This may contribute to patients feeling more prepared for health discussions and decisions with their clinicians. Moreover, patients with a lengthy medical history may have established relationships and trust with healthcare providers. This can facilitate discussions on personal issues and foster a balanced power dynamic during consultations. Our data did not allow for an analysis of how the relationship duration or the number of previous appointments influenced the level of perceived embarrassment and power asymmetry. Thus, these considerations require further research. This is particularly interesting because familiarity and trust offer potential targets for interventions to reduce interpersonal barriers to SDM.

Our results are based on self-report data, a common method to assess patient participation. However, self-report data may be biased by social desirability. Although participants were informed that their answers would not be accessible to their clinician, it is plausible that they were still hesitant to report embarrassment, power asymmetry, or decisional conflict in or after the consultation with their clinician. This might help explain the comparably low levels of these measures in the patient sample.

When measuring patients’ participation preferences before consultations in which high-stake decisions are made, patients may not yet be fully aware of their options and are forced to estimate their preferences [92]. Also, when completing questionnaires before the consultation, patients may be in a less emotionally aroused state (cold) compared to the actual discussion about their healthcare journey (hot). This may create a disparity between patients’ hypothetical and actual preferences, referred to as the hot-to-cold empathy gap [70, 93]. However, the case vignettes in the API-Uro questionnaire represent common decisions in urological care [63], which might have reduced biases in the reported participation preferences. Also, we assessed participation preferences shortly before the decision-making situation to minimize the impact of an empathy gap on the general participation preference measure (API).

In our regression models predicting participation preference and decisional conflict, power asymmetry and embarrassment explained only a small proportion of variance. However, participation preference and decisional conflict are multifaceted phenomena, and one cannot expect a single factor to explain large amounts of variance. Previous studies examining predictors of participation preferences and decisional conflict have found comparable proportions of explained variance (participation preference: adjusted R2 = 0.03 − 0.25; [94,95,96]; decisional conflict: adjusted R2 = 0.1 − 0.12; [59]).

Practical implications

SDM fosters choices that align with the values and unique needs of patients. However, simply introducing SDM in consultations may be ineffective. SDM consultations differ considerably from the appointments many patients are used to, and a substantial part of patients might have no SDM experience [97]. Thus, patients need to be prepared, with particular caution to the impact of power asymmetry and embarrassment on patients’ capability to engage in decisions.

Current interventions in general healthcare [87] and urology [31] do not address power asymmetries in the patient-clinician-relationship. Patients’ perceptions of their role as inferior in the decision-making process must be challenged, and their concept of good (passive and compliant) patients must be redefined. Explicit encouragement is an effective facilitator of involvement in SDM [98, 99]. Thus, patients should explicitly be encouraged to step out of their passive role and engage in critical conversations [35]. Clinicians reassuring patients that active participation is valuable and will not lead to negative outcomes can be a simple yet effective intervention. Online self-help groups can further reduce negative attitudes [100]. Pre-consultation interventions, like coaching sessions and patient activation [101, 102] to foster patients’ self-efficacy and confidence, have been embedded, but their integration into routine healthcare is challenging due to time constraints [35]. Therefore, alternative methods should be investigated. As power dynamics involve both patient and clinician, interventions should also target clinicians. Providers can use the PA-ME questionnaire to assess power asymmetry as perceived by their patients. This may raise awareness and encourage clinicians to reduce power asymmetry in their consultations.

Patients who experience heightened levels of embarrassment require exceptional support and active involvement. A strong patient-clinician relationship, coupled with effective communication skills of the clinician, can help patients overcome embarrassment when discussing intimate topics [103]. Thus, providers should invest time in building a trusting relationship with patients. Active listening, empathy, and respect are essential to facilitate open communication. Clinicians can also openly acknowledge that embarrassment in medical settings is common, thereby normalizing patients’ experiences. Training programs to enhance communication skills, build trustworthy relationships, and create supportive environments should be implemented into clinicians’ curricula. Healthcare services can educate patients about what to expect during medical encounters, including the procedures involved and what will be expected of them. Institutional approaches that incorporate SDM principles into oncological care may also help alleviate fears and concerns.

Conclusions

Our findings confirm that power asymmetry and embarrassment in the medical setting are relevant aspects in patient-clinician interactions. We provide instruments to reliably measure the constructs in future research and in routine care. To enable patients to actively participate in important medical decisions with potentially life-altering consequences, it’s crucial to address both interpersonal dynamics through practical interventions. Ultimately, this may help ensure patients’ autonomy and self-determination and lead to improved health outcomes and patient satisfaction.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study will be deposited on MADATA (University of Mannheim, https://madata.bib.uni-mannheim.de/) Research Data Repository and made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher. The final versions of the EmMed and the PA-ME questionnaire will be available in German and English on MADOC (University of Mannheim, https://madoc.bib.uni-mannheim.de/) Research Output Repository.

Abbreviations

SDM:

Shared decision–making

PA-ME:

Power Asymmetry in Medical Encounters

EmMed:

Embarrassment in Medical Consultation

API:

Autonomy Preference Index

API-Uro:

Autonomy Preference Index for Urooncology

DCS:

Decisional Conflict Scale

EFA:

Exploratory factor analysis

KMO:

Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure of sample adequacy

VSS criterion:

Very simple structure criterion

MAP test:

Velicer’s minimum average partial test

References

  1. Barry MJ, Edgman-Levitan S. Shared decision making - pinnacle of patient-centered care. N Engl J Med. 2012;366(9):780–1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Wennberg JE. Unwarranted variations in healthcare delivery: implications for academic medical centres. BMJ. 2002;325(7370):961–4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Jani AB, Hellman S. Early prostate cancer: clinical decision-making. Lancet. 2003;361(9362):1045–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Sepucha KR, Fowler FJ, Mulley AG. Policy support for patient-centered care: the need for measurable improvements in decision quality. Health Aff (Millwood) 2004; (23 SUPPL.2.):VAR54–62.

  5. Politi MC, Dizon DS, Frosch DL, Kuzemchak MD, Stiggelbout AM. Importance of clarifying patients’ desired role in shared decision making to match their level of engagement with their preferences. BMJ. 2013;347:f7066.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Charles C, Gafni A, Whelan T. Shared decision-making in the medical encounter: what does it mean? (or it takes at least two to tango). Soc Sci Med. 1997;44(5):681–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Stacey D, Légaré F, Lewis KB, Barry MJ, Bennett CL, Eden KB, et al. Decision aids for people facing health treatment or screening decisions. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2017;4(4):CD001431.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. van Stam M-A, Pieterse AH, van der Poel HG, Bosch JLHR, Tillier C, Horenblas S, et al. Shared decision making in prostate Cancer Care-Encouraging every patient to be actively involved in decision making or ensuring the patient preferred level of involvement?? J Urol. 2018;200(3):582–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Arbuthnott A, Sharpe D. The effect of physician-patient collaboration on patient adherence in non-psychiatric medicine. Patient Educ Couns. 2009;77(1):60–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Kahn KL, Schneider EC, Malin JL, Adams JL, Epstein AM. Patient centered experiences in breast cancer: predicting long-term adherence to Tamoxifen use. Med Care. 2007;45(5):431–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Wilson SR, Strub P, Buist AS, Knowles SB, Lavori PW, Lapidus J, et al. Shared treatment decision making improves adherence and outcomes in poorly controlled asthma. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2010;181(6):566–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Gattellari M, Butow PN, Tattersall MHN. Sharing decisions in cancer care. Soc Sci Med. 2001;52(12):1865–78.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Luo H, Liu G, Lu J, Di Xue. Association of shared decision making with inpatient satisfaction: a cross-sectional study. BMC Med Inf Decis Mak. 2021;21(1):25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Nakayama K, Osaka W, Matsubara N, Takeuchi T, Toyoda M, Ohtake N, et al. Shared decision making, physicians’ explanations, and treatment satisfaction: a cross-sectional survey of prostate cancer patients. BMC Med Inf Decis Mak. 2020;20(1):334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Beauchamp TL, Childress JF. Principles of biomedical ethics. Oxford University Press; 2001.

  16. German Ethics Council. Patientenwohl als ethischer Maßstab für das Krankenhaus - Stellungnahme [Patient well-being as an ethical benchmark for the hospital - statement]; 2016 Apr 5 [cited 2023 Mar 21]. Available from: URL: https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1515/jwiet-2017-0016/html

  17. Bravo P, Härter M, McCaffery K, Giguère A, Hahlweg P, Elwyn G. Editorial: 20 years after the start of international shared Decision-Making activities: is it time to celebrate? Probably… Z Evid Fortbild Qual Gesundhwes. 2022;171:1–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Nationaler Krebsplan. Handlungsfelder, Ziele, Umsetzungsempfehlungen und Ergebnisse. [National Cancer Plan - Areas of action, goals, implementation recommendations and results]; 2017. Available from: URL: https://www.bundesgesundheitsministerium.de/fileadmin/Dateien/5_Publikationen/Praevention/Broschueren/Broschuere_Nationaler_Krebsplan.pdf

  19. Hahlweg P, Kriston L, Scholl I, Brähler E, Faller H, Schulz H, et al. Cancer patients’ preferred and perceived level of involvement in treatment decision-making: an epidemiological study. Acta Oncol. 2020;59(8):967–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Noteboom EA, May AM, van der Wall E, de Wit NJ, Helsper CW. Patients’ preferred and perceived level of involvement in decision making for cancer treatment: A systematic review. Psychooncology. 2021;30(10):1663–79.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Tariman JD, Berry DL, Cochrane B, Doorenbos A, Schepp K. Preferred and actual participation roles during health care decision making in persons with cancer: a systematic review. Ann Oncol. 2010;21(6):1145–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Kriegmair MC, Alpers GW. Partizipative entscheidungsfindung Bei der harnableitung [Shared decision-making in urinary diversion]. Im Fokus Onkologie. 2020;23(6):59–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Ong LM, de Haes JCJM, Hoos AM, Lammes FB. Doctor-patient communication: a review of the literature. Soc Sci Med. 1995;40(7):903–18.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Büdenbender B, Köther AK, Grüne B, Michel MS, Kriegmair MC, Alpers GW. When attitudes and beliefs get in the way of shared decision-making: A mediation analysis of participation preference. Health Expect. 2023;26(2):740–51.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Orom H, Biddle C, Underwood W, Nelson CJ, Homish DL. What is a good treatment decision?? decision?al control, knowledge, treatment decision? making, and quality of life in men with clinically localized prostate Cancer. Med Decis Mak. 2016;36(6):714–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Burnett AL, Nehra A, Breau RH, Culkin DJ, Faraday MM, Hakim LS, et al. Erectile dysfunction: AUA guideline. J Urol. 2018;200(3):633–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Sanda MG, Cadeddu JA, Kirkby E, Chen RC, Crispino T, Fontanarosa J, et al. Clinically localized prostate cancer: AUA/ASTRO/SUO guideline. Part I: risk stratification, shared decision making, and care options. J Urol. 2018;199(3):683–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Tikkinen KAO, Dahm P, Lytvyn L, Heen AF, Vernooij RWM, Siemieniuk RAC, et al. Prostate cancer screening with prostate-specific antigen (PSA) test: a clinical practice guideline. BMJ. 2018;362:k3581.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Groeben C, Baunacke M, Borkowetz A, Kliesch S, Wülfing C, Ihrig A, et al. Entscheidungshilfen für patienten Finden Bei Deutschen urologen Breite Akzeptanz: umfrage unter mitgliedern von DGU und BDU. Urologe A. 2016;55(6):784–91.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Hahlweg P, Hoffmann J, Härter M, Frosch DL, Elwyn G, Scholl I. In absentia: an exploratory study of how patients are considered in multidisciplinary Cancer team meetings. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(10):e0139921.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Grüne B, Kriegmair MC, Lenhart M, Michel MS, Huber J, Köther AK, et al. Decision aids for shared Decision-making in Uro-oncology: A systematic review. Eur Urol Focus. 2022;8(3):851–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Scholl I, LaRussa A, Hahlweg P, Kobrin S, Elwyn G. Organizational- and system-level characteristics that influence implementation of shared decision-making and strategies to address them - a scoping review. Implement Sci. 2018;13(1):40.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Loonen JJ, Blijlevens NM, Prins J, Dona DJ, den Hartogh J, Senden T, et al. Cancer survivorship care: person centered care in a multidisciplinary shared care model. Int J Integr Care. 2018;18(1):4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Surbone A, Lowenstein J. Exploring asymmetry in the relationship between patients and physicians. J Clin Ethics. 2003;14(3):183–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Joseph-Williams N, Elwyn G, Edwards AGK. Knowledge is not power for patients: a systematic review and thematic synthesis of patient-reported barriers and facilitators to shared decision making. Patient Educ Couns. 2014;94(3):291–309.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Frosch DL, May SG, Rendle KAS, Tietbohl C, Elwyn G. Authoritarian physicians and patients’ fear of being labeled ‘difficult’ among key Obstacles to shared decision making. Health Aff (Millwood). 2012;31(5):1030–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Charles C, Whelan T, Gafni A, Reyno L, Redko C. Doing nothing is no choice: Lay constructions of treatment Decision-making among women with Early‐stage breast Cancer. Sociol Health Illn. 1998;20(1):71–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Adams JR, Elwyn G, Légaré F, Frosch DL. Communicating with physicians about medical decisions: a reluctance to disagree. Arch Intern Med. 2012;172(15):1184–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Mazor KM, Roblin DW, Greene SM, Lemay CA, Firneno CL, Calvi J, et al. Toward patient-centered cancer care: patient perceptions of problematic events, impact, and response. J Clin Oncol. 2012;30(15):1784–90.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Yoong W, Assassi Z, Ahmedani I, Abdinasir R, Denning M, Taylor H, et al. Why are patients not more involved in their own safety? A questionnaire-based survey in a multi-ethnic North London hospital population. Postgrad Med J. 2019;95(1123):266–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Judson TJ, Detsky AS, Press MJ. Encouraging patients to ask questions: how to overcome white-coat silence. JAMA. 2013;309(22):2325–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Berry LL, Danaher TS, Beckham D, Awdish RLA, Mate KS. When patients and their families feel like hostages to health care. Mayo Clin Proc. 2017;92(9):1373–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Arora NK, Ayanian JZ, Guadagnoli E. Examining the relationship of patients’ attitudes and beliefs with their self-reported level of participation in medical decision-making. Med Care. 2005;43(9):865–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Ort A, Ekel. Wut Sowie verlegenheit, scham und Schuld in der gesundheitskommunikation [Disgust, anger as well as embarrassment, shame and guilt in health communication]. In: Rossmann C, Hastall MR, editors. Handbuch der gesundheitskommunikation. Springer VS; 2019. pp. 447–57.

  45. Edelmann RJ. Social embarrassment: an analysis of the process. J Social Personal Relationships. 1985;2(2):195–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Buss AH. Self-consciousness and social anxiety. WH freeman; 1980.

  47. Meerabeau L. The management of embarrassment and sexuality in health care. J Adv Nurs. 1999;29(6):1507–13.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Shaw C, Williams K, Assassa PR, Jackson C. Patient satisfaction with urodynamics: a qualitative study. J Adv Nurs. 2000;32(6):1356–63.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Veit I, Spiekermann K. Dealing with shame in a medical context. In: Mayer C-H, Vanderheiden E, editors. The bright side of shame: transforming and growing through practical applications in cultural contexts. Cham: Springer; 2019. pp. 349–61.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  50. Parrott WG, Sabini J, Silver M. The roles of Self-Esteem and social interaction in embarrassment. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 1988;14(1):191–202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Dolezal L, Lyons B. Health-related shame: an affective determinant of health? Medical Humanities. 2017; 43(4):257–63. Available from: URL: https://mh.bmj.com/content/43/4/257?fbclid=iwar38xvwmlpxhydppit85pyeyhn_c3rqhjkc0z79j929ipbu-khnfgg4t8ie

  52. Harris CR, Darby RS. Shame in Physician–Patient interactions: patient perspectives. Basic Appl Soc Psychol. 2009;31(4):325–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Hägglund D, Walker-Engström M-L, Larsson G, Leppert J. Reasons why women with long-term urinary incontinence do not seek professional help: a cross-sectional population-based cohort study. Int Urogynecol J Pelvic Floor Dysfunct. 2003; 14(5):296–304; discussion 304. Available from: URL: https://link.springer.com/article/https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s00192-003-1077-9

  54. Roberts RO, Rhodes T, Panser LA, Girman CJ, Chute CG, Oesterling JE et al. Natural history of prostatism: worry and embarrassment from urinary symptoms and health care-seeking behavior. Urology. 1994; 43(5):621–8. Available from: URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/0090429594901740

  55. Consedine NS, Krivoshekova YS, Harris CR. Bodily embarrassment and judgment concern as separable factors in the measurement of medical embarrassment: psychometric development and links to treatment-seeking outcomes. Br J Health Psychol. 2007;12(3):439–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Scheel CN, Bender C, Tuschen-Caffier B, Jacob GA. SHAME– Entwicklung eines Fragebogens Zur erfassung positiver und negativer aspekte von scham [SHAME - Development of a questionnaire capturing positive and negative aspects of shame]. Z Für Klinische Psychologie Und Psychother. 2013;42(4):280–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Farahani MA, Sahragard R, Carroll JK, Mohammadi E. Communication barriers to patient education in cardiac inpatient care: a qualitative study of multiple perspectives. Int J Nurs Pract. 2011;17(3):322–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Barr PJ, Elwyn G. Measurement challenges in shared decision making: putting the ‘patient’ in patient-reported measures. Health Expect. 2016;19(5):993–1001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Köther AK, Alpers GW, Büdenbender B, Lenhart M, Michel MS, Kriegmair MC. Predicting decisional conflict: Anxiety and depression in shared decision making. Patient Educ Couns. 2021; 104(5):1229–36. Available from: URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/s0738399120305991

  60. Kryworuchko J, Stacey D, Bennett CL, Graham ID. Appraisal of primary outcome measures used in trials of patient decision support. Patient Educ Couns. 2008; 73(3):497–503. Available from: URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/s0738399108003455

  61. Ende J, Kazis L, Ash A, Moskowitz MA. Measuring patients’ desire for autonomy: decision making and information-seeking preferences among medical patients. J Gen Intern Med. 1989; 4(1):23–30. Available from: URL: https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/bf02596485

  62. Simon D, Härter M, Kriston L. Die deutsche modifizierte Fassung des Autonomie-Präferenz-Index (API-Dm) [The German modified version of the Autonomy Preference Index (API-Dm)]. Klinische Diagnostik und Evaluation. 2011; 4(1):5–14. Available from: URL: http://patient-als-partner.de/media/simon_2011_-_api-dm.pdf

  63. Büdenbender B, Köther AK, Kriegmair MC, Grüne B, Michel MS, Alpers GW. Getting specific: participation preference in urooncological decision-making. BMC Med Inf Decis Mak 2023; 23(1).

  64. Buchholz A, Hölzel LP, Kriston L, Simon D, Härter M. Die Decisional Conflict Scale in deutscher Sprache (DCS-D) - Dimensionale Struktur in einer Stichprobe von Hausarztpatienten. [The Decisional Conflict Scale in German (DCS-D) - Dimensional Structure in a Sample of Primary Care Patients]. Klinische Diagnostik und Evaluation. 2011; 4:15–30. Available from: URL: http://www.patient-als-partner.de/media/kde_04_01_buchholz_etal.pdf

  65. Leiner DJ. SoSci Survey [Computer software]. 2019. https://www.soscisurvey.de

  66. SurveyCircle., Forschungswebseite. SurveyCircle; 2023. Available from: URL: https://www.surveycircle.com

  67. Sona Systems. Cloud-based Participant Management Software. Available from: URL: https://www.sona-systems.com/

  68. Yamaguchi T, Ota K. Development of the inpatient attitudes towards the patient role scale. Japan J Nurs Sci. 2012;9(1):88–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Mayer C-H, Vanderheiden E, editors. The bright side of shame: transforming and growing through practical applications in cultural contexts. Cham: Springer; 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Chewning B, Bylund CL, Shah B, Arora NK, Gueguen JA, Makoul G. Patient preferences for shared decisions: a systematic review. Patient Educ Couns. 2012;86(1):9–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Scholl I, Koelewijn-van Loon M, Sepucha KR, Elwyn G, Légaré F, Härter M et al. Measurement of shared decision making - a review of instruments. Z Evid Fortbild Qual Gesundhwes. 2011; 105(4):313–24. Available from: URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/s1865921711001218

  72. Köther AK, Büdenbender B, Grüne B, Holbach S, Huber J, von Landenberg N, et al. Different patients, different preferences: A multicenter assessment of patients’ personality traits and anxiety in shared decision making. Cancer Med. 2022;11(15):2999–3008.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Kates JM. Treatment-related Decisional Conflict, Quality of Life, and Comorbidity in Older Adults with Cancer. Asia-Pacific Journal of Oncology Nursing. 2018; 5(4):421–9. Available from: URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/s2347562521003127

  74. Thompson-Leduc P, Turcotte S, Labrecque M, Légaré F. Prevalence of clinically significant decisional conflict: an analysis of five studies on decision-making in primary care. BMJ Open. 2016;6(6):e011490.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Garvelink MM, Boland L, Klein K, Nguyen DV, Menear M, Bekker HL, et al. Decisional conflict scale use over 20 years: the anniversary review. Med Decis Mak. 2019;39(4):301–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Brown TA. Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York: The Guilford Press; 2006. (Methodology in the Social Sciences).

    Google Scholar 

  77. Kaiser HF, Rice J, Little, Jiffy. Mark Iv. Educ Psychol Meas. 1974;34(1):111–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Costello AB, Osborne J. Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: Four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Practical Assessment, Research, and Evaluation. 2005; (10). Available from: URL: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/pare/vol10/iss1/7/

  79. Watkins MW. Exploratory factor analysis: A guide to best practice. J Black Psychol. 2018;44(3):219–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Zygmont C, Smith MR. Robust factor analysis in the presence of normality violations, missing data, and outliers: empirical questions and possible solutions. Quant Methods Psychol. 2014;10(1):40–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Gadermann AM, Guhn M, Zumbo BD et al. Estimating ordinal reliability for Likert-type and ordinal item response data: A conceptumpirical, and practical guide. Practical Assessment, Research, and Evaluation. 2019; 17(1). Available from: URL: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/pare/vol17/iss1/3/

  82. Zumbo BD, Gadermann AM, Zeisser C. Ordinal versions of coefficients alpha and Theta for likert rating scales. J Mod Appl Stat Methods. 2007;6(1):21–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull. 1992;112(1):155–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. RStudio PBC. 2023. Available from: URL: http://www.rstudio.com/

  85. Hoelzle JB, Meyer GJ. Exploratory Factor Analysis: Basics and Beyond. In: Weiner I, editor. Handbook of Psychology, Second Edition. Wiley; 2012.

  86. Warne RT, Larsen R. Evaluating a proposed modification of the Guttman rule for determining the number of factors in an exploratory factor analysis. Psychological Test and Assessment Modeling. 2014; 56(1):104–23. Available from: URL: https://www.researchgate.net/profile/ross-larsen/publication/284548804_evaluating_a_proposed_modification_of_the_guttman_rule_for_determining_the_number_of_factors_in_an_exploratory_factor_analysis

  87. Joseph-Williams N, Edwards AGK, Elwyn G. Power imbalance prevents shared decision making. BMJ. 2014;348:g3178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Hamann J, Bühner M, Rüsch N. Self-Stigma and consumer participation in shared decision making in mental health services. Psychiatr Serv. 2017;68(8):783–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Shay LA, Lafata JE. Where is the evidence? A systematic review of shared decision making and patient outcomes. Med Decis Mak. 2015;35(1):114–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Robert Koch Institute, Association of Population Based Cancer Registries in Germany. Krebs in Deutschland in 2015/2016 [Cancer in Germany in 2015/16]; 2019 [cited 2023 May 18]. Available from: URL: https://www.krebsdaten.de/Krebs/DE/Content/Publikationen/Krebs_in_Deutschland/kid_2019/krebs_in_deutschland_2019.pdf?__blob=publicationFile

  91. Schönbrodt FD, Perugini M. At what sample size do correlations stabilize? J Res Pers. 2013;47(5):609–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Elwyn G, Edwards AGK, Kinnersley P, Grol R. Shared decision making and the concept of equipoise: the competences of involving patients in healthcare choices. British Journal of General Practice. 2000; 50(460):892–9. Available from: URL: https://bjgp.org/content/50/460/892.short

  93. Loewenstein G. Hot-cold empathy gaps and medical decision making. Health Psychol. 2005;24(4S):49–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  94. Hamann J, Neuner B, Kasper J, Vodermaier A, Loh A, Deinzer A, et al. Participation preferences of patients with acute and chronic conditions. Health Expect. 2007;10(4):358–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  95. Köther AK, Siebenhaar KU, Alpers GW. Shared decision making during the COVID-19 pandemic. Med Decis Mak. 2021;41(4):430–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  96. Mattukat K, Boehm P, Raberger K, Schaefer C, Keyszer G, Mau W. How much information and participation do patients with inflammatory rheumatic diseases prefer in interaction with physicians?? Results of a participatory research project. Patient Prefer Adherence. 2019;13:2145–58.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  97. Braun B, Marstedt G. Partizipative entscheidungsfindung beim Arzt: anspruch und wirklichkeit. [Shared decision-making with the physician: theory and practice]. Gesundheitsmonitor. 2014;31(2):107–31.

    Google Scholar 

  98. Bastiaens H, van Royen P, Pavlic DR, Raposo V, Baker R. Older people’s preferences for involvement in their own care: a qualitative study in primary health care in 11 European countries. Patient Educ Couns. 2007; 68(1):33–42. Available from: URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/s0738399107001449

  99. Sainio C, Lauri S, Eriksson E. Cancer patients’ views and experiences of participation in care and decision making. Nurs Ethics. 2001;8(2):97–113.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Winzelberg AJ, Classen C, Alpers GW, Roberts H, Koopman C, Adams RE, et al. Evaluation of an internet support group for women with primary breast cancer. Cancer. 2003;97(5):1164–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Brown RF, Butow PN, Sharrock MA, Henman M, Boyle F, Goldstein D, et al. Education and role modelling for clinical decisions with female cancer patients. Health Expect. 2004;7(4):303–16.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  102. Shepherd HL, Barratt A, Trevena LJ, McGeechan K, Carey K, Epstein RM et al. Three questions that patients can ask to improve the quality of information physicians give about treatment options: a cross-over trial. Patient Educ Couns. 2011; 84(3):379–85. Available from: URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/s0738399111003892

  103. Eliacin J, Salyers MP, Kukla M, Matthias MS. Factors influencing patients’ preferences and perceived involvement in shared decision-making in mental health care. J Ment Health. 2015;24(1):24–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all individuals who participated in the studies and the study nurses who supported the recruitment. We would like to thank Dr. Sarah K. Danböck for her help with editing a previous version of the manuscript.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.

Financial support for this study was provided by the German Cancer Aid (Deutsche Krebshilfe) funding program Patient Orientation (grant number: 7011323). The funding agreement ensured the authors’ independence in designing the study, interpreting the data, and writing the report. The publication of this article was funded by the Mannheim Centre for European Social Research (MZES).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

KAS substantially contributed to the conception and design of the study, acquisition of data, data analysis and interpretation, and drafting and revising of the manuscript. BB substantially contributed to the conception and design of the study, data interpretation, and revising of the manuscript. AKB substantially contributed to the conception and design of the study and revising of the manuscript. BG substantially contributed to the conception and design of the study and acquisition of data. MCK substantially contributed to the acquisition of data. MSM substantially contributed to the funding. GWA substantially contributed to the conception and design of the study, acquisition of funding, data interpretation, and revising of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Georg W. Alpers.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study on the urological sample was approved by the ethics committee of the Medical Faculty of Mannheim, University of Heidelberg (amendment to MA-2019–635N) and conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants of both, the urological as well as the non-patient sample, were informed about the study and provided written informed consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Scherer, K.A., Büdenbender, B., Blum, A.K. et al. Power asymmetry and embarrassment in shared decision-making: predicting participation preference and decisional conflict. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak 25, 120 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12911-025-02938-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12911-025-02938-4

Keywords